Skip to main content

Review of 'Explicit Memory for Unattended Words: The Importance of Being in the "No"'

Typically, young adults who are asked to ignore distracting information (i.e. inhibit it), are quite capable of doing exactly that.  They tend to perform well on the attended task and show little to no memory for items that are presented as distraction (which they were asked to ignore).  This has usually been taken as evidence that when tested implicitly for encoding distractors, young adults under normal conditions demonstrate excellent ability to filter these out and prevent access to working memory (i.e. Gazzaley et al., 2008; Lustig, Hasher, & Zacks, 2007).

The upshot is that young adults supposedly are more able to focus on what matters (the task at hand), but may not have access to the information they were asked to block out since it was supposedly filtered out before it could be incorporated into a memory trace.

This is where things get interesting...  Hoffman et al., claim that in fact this type of information is indeed accessible - scientists just haven't been using sensitive enough measures to evoke it.  The group used a task developed by Rees et al., (1999) where words were superimposed on line drawings and participants were asked to respond every time a line drawing repeated (ignoring the words).  Rees et al., showed that young adults successfully ignored the words unless they were specifically asked to attend them.

From Rees et al., (1999)





From Hoffman et al., (2011)


Hoffman et al., (2011) used a refinement of this study by adding confidence ratings on top of pure recognition.  In other words, not only were participants being asked "do you recall seeing this word?", but "how confident (using a 4 point scale) are you about that response?"

As you can see (above) using this new measure, the authors managed to look within the "No" responses (at words that were supposedly inhibited) and show that people are more confident that novel words are actually new than words that previously appeared as distraction meaning that young adults do in fact have access to this information at some level.

This is quite an impressive finding.

The authors then go on to claim that this is explicit memory.  Here I think they may be overstating their claims a bit.  True, recognition of items is explicit and requires a conscious memory to make that judgement. However claiming that items that were consciously rejected but show what I would argue is an implicit difference using a confidence rating is another kettle of fish altogether.  What the authors have, is a replication of the previously described explicit findings, but within them an implicit difference.

In the inhibition and attention literature, this is a very significant finding as it demonstrates that previously inhibited information isn't lost, but may be accessed given the right circumstance - that said, I would argue that this information is almost certainly not explicit, and would probably be more akin to an implicit "gut feeling" than anything else.


Gazzaley, A., Clapp, W., Kelley, J., McEvoy, K., Knight, R. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2008). Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(35), 13122-6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0806074105
Hoffman, Y., Bein, O., & Maril, A. (2011). Explicit memory for unattended words: the importance of being in the “no”. Psychological science, 22(12), 1490-3. doi:10.1177/0956797611419674
Lustig, C., Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (2007). Inhibitory Deficit Theory : Recent Developments in a “ New View .” Inhibition in cognition, (571), 145–162. American Psychological Association Washington, DC. Retrieved from http://www-personal.umich.edu/~clustig/Publications/chapters/LustigHasherZacks.pdf
Rees, G., Russell, C., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (1999). Inattentional blindness versus inattentional amnesia for fixated but ignored words. Science, 286(5449), 2504-2507. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10617465

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Video games allow for multitasking? A review of "Improving multi-tasking ability through action videogames"

W ould you believe that playing video games could make you more likely to succeed as a pilot? There's been quite a bit of colloquial evidence on multitasking and task-switching floating around the internet recently, thanks in no small part to work by Ophir et al., (2009) who showed that people who are habitual media-multitaskers (i.e. those annoying people who text while watching a movie or even worse while driving) tend to perform worse on a wide variety of cognitive measures of attention.  They concluded that people who are chronic media multitaskers have a "leaky filter", and as such cannot block out irrelevant (and distracting) information.  This agrees nicely other theories of attention, and particularly inhibitory theory (Hasher & Zacks, Michael C. Anderson, Gazzaley). One reason that multitasking has come under such scrutiny is that it is now encouraged and even expected in many professions and by many employers.  Even in situations where multitasking...

Dopamine helps the brain change gears: A review of "Dopamine-supports coupling of attention-related networks."

Before we begin, some background information... It seems that networks and the idea of connectivity is on everybody's minds these days (pardon the pun).  It's a fairly safe bet to say that nearly everyone affiliated with neuroscience has more than a passing familiarity with the default mode and dorsal attention networks (i.e. see Raichle et al., 2001).  These networks are anticorrelated which is to say that when one is being more heavily accessed, the activity in the other is suppressed.  Broadly, the default mode is active at rest, during mind wandering and when thinking about the autobiographical past - it has been described as being related to internally directed attention.  The dorsal attention or task positive network on the other hand is engaged when attention needs to be directed outwards at a cognitively demanding task or situation.  From Dang et al., 2012 What about the frontoparietal cognitive control network?  This network was discove...

On the use and abuse of Hairballs. What might graph-theory mean for cognitive neuroscience?

Olaf Sporns Discovering the Human Connectome H aving just returned from the OHBM (Organization for Human Brain Mapping) it's not hard to be phenomenally ecstatic about the state of research and raring to apply new techniques and theories to my data.   There is much to be hopeful for; I encountered many brilliant people who are excited about their research and how neuroscience is helping to unlock the secrets of the mind. That said, there did seem to be a great deal of research that was following the coat-tails of last year's big ideas.  This year the "big thing" was graph theory - the branch of mathematics and computer science that was previously applied to other fields such as social network visualization and biology  by Olaf Sporns and applied to brain networks.   Graph theory works on the notion that you can reduce a matrix (normally a correlation matrix) to a set of nodes (which represent specific brain regions) and edges (which represent either phys...